Kids who lack a basic number concept when
they start school dont do well in math. Can we teach them the
concept?
By Rosemarie Bahr
Five Grade
1 students are playing a game. Theyre delivering letters in
a neighbourhood that consists of a line of 40 cardboard rowhouses.
The houses all have numbers on their doors, from 1 to 40. The first
10 houses have the same colour doors. The second 10 doors are a different
colour. The even-numbered roofs are flat. The odd-numbered roofs are
pointed. The kids are finding different ways to get to house 34 from
house 15. Theyre having fun, and theyre learning math.

Robbie Case
|
This is part
of Rightstart, developed during the research carried out by
Robbie Case, a fellow in the Human Development Program of the
Canadian
Institute for Advanced Research
and professor at the University of Toronto.
Our kids dont
do as well as they should
International comparisons
show that North American children dont do as well in
math as their Asian peers. Young Brazilian street vendors,
children with no formal schooling, also do quite well compared
to Canadian children.
|
Other studies reveal that North
American children have a systematic misunderstanding of some math
concepts they have been taught. For Robbie Case, these problems indicate
a need to reshape the teaching of math.
Case and his colleagues first
sought to discover how children learn math. They found that children
develop a "central conceptual structure," which is the basis
for further learning. Children who do not have this number central
conceptual structure, or basic number concept, by the time they get
to school, do not do well in math.
These students do not develop
a good number sense, which would let them move between the real world
of quantities and the mathematical world of numbers and symbols. Students
who have good number sense can invent their own procedures and recognize
and use number patterns. They can recognize gross errors, and they
can represent a single number in many ways.
For example, suppose you have
19 marbles and someone gives you 15 more. How many would you have
all together?
Many children, and adults for
that matter, line up the 19 and 15 in their minds, the way they were
taught. They add the nine and the five to get 14 and carry the one.
Then they add that one to the one and one to get three, ending up
with 34.
People with good number sense,
however, intuitively invent an easier procedure on the spot. It might
be to add 15 and 20 to get 35, then subtract one to get 34. Another
method would be to add 15 and 15 to get 30 and then add four to get
34.
Both methods are based on an understanding
of the base 10 nature of our number system. In the second procedure,
19 is shown as 20 minus one. This change displays a confidence that
their own procedure is valid and shows an attitude that the students
have somehow made mathematics their own.
Can basic number concept
and good number sense be learned?
Case sees a connection between
numerical and spatial knowledge. Mathematicians often say their intuitions
have a strong spatial component. Adults with brain injuries that impede
spatial understanding often also have difficulty with numbers. Children
who are good at reading but poor at math also often show weaker spatial
abilities.
If there is a connection, it should
be possible to create a learning experience in which numbers are linked
to space and children can use their spatial intuitions to learn about
numbers.

Students enjoy inventing their own equations
as they
deliver the mail with Rightstart.
Case and his colleague Sharon
Griffin developed a program, Rightstart, to create that learning experience.
They first tried it in Toronto in the late 80s with a group
of children from three kindergarten classes serving middle to low-income
families who had immigrated from rural Portugal. The second study
took place in kindergartens in three schools in Massachusetts. These
schools had the largest proportion of visible minority students in
the city and generally drew from low to middle-low-income families.
These and later trials showed
that children who went through the program demonstrated increased
number sense compared to a control group. The improved number sense
also showed up in related contexts such as science, telling time and
dealing with money.
When the children were followed
up at the end of Grade 1, they were doing much better at arithmetic
than the control group. When the program expanded and was added to
the first and second grade curriculum, it improved the responses in
an entire population at risk for school failure. The Rightstart group
became the equivalent of a more advantaged population in most respects,
even superior in some.
Over the years, Case and Griffin
worked on refining the games and components of Rightstart. The program
is being translated into French for use in some downtown Montreal
schools. Griffin, who is now at Clark University in Massachusetts,
has been working on workshops for teachers to help them feel comfortable
with the materials.
How does Rightstart work?
One of the keys to Rightstart
is the spatial embodiment of numbers and number systems. The numbers
stay fixed, as they are on the rowhouses in the neighbourhood.
"When youre playing
a board game," says Case, "the numbers are all laid out
and you move through them. Math in the early grades can include bundling
things, like popsicle sticks, into groups of 10 or counting with counters.
But that doesnt give a sense of how each number relates to each
other number, that the teens belong together and 23 is like 33 in
certain ways."
According to Case, children should
have one or two contexts in which to explore the math problems. In
the neighbourhood of row houses, the context remains the same, but
15 or 20 different games are played there. As the children learn,
more houses are attached, using velcro, until there are 100.
Fun and storytelling are crucial.
Case reports that both the children and the teacher have fun. The
activities are part of a story, like delivering letters in the neighbourhood
or going down a line of boxes to put out the fire of the dragon who
lives in a box at the end and is terrorizing the city.
Ownership and diversity are embedded
in the program. One child may use her 10-step boots to hop from house
15 to house 25, then to house 35, then walk back one to house 34 so
she can deliver her letter. Another child may use his boots to hop
to house 25, then walk nine steps to house 34 to deliver his letter.
Both students will tell the group
about their methods. They may discuss the advantages and disadvantages
and decide there arent any.
By Grade 1 or 2, Case reports,
these kids are inventing their own equations. Just as there is no
best single composition or drawing in a class, there is no one best
equation. And they can all go up on the wall or home to parents. The
children feel ownership. They dont feel there is only one right
mathematical expression. In that way theyre more like mathematicians
and less like adding machines.
Moving on to fractions
Rational numbers (fractions, decimals,
per cents) in Grades 5 and 6 are often a roadblock for students at
the middle and top end of the socio-economic scale. In a national
multiple-choice test, the majority of American children in Grade 12,
when asked to estimate the sum of 9/10 and 11/12, picked 19 or 21
as the answer instead of two. Students have some serious misconceptions
about fractions.
With Joan Moss, a teacher and
lecturer at the University of Torontos Institute for Child Study,
Case has been working on another set of spatial embodiments that starts
with per cents rather than fractions, which fits better with childrens
developing intuitions about proportions. This has been tried several
times with advantaged students in Grades 4 to 6.
"If I said, what is
65 per cent of 160? most people would say, Excuse me,
you expect me to do that in my head? Most of the kids whove
come through the program have no problem with that. Its like
asking them whats nine and 32. Its a bit challenging,
but not intimidating."
If Robbie Case has his way, all
our children will be included in the information age.
The
Canadian Institute for Advanced Research
The Canadian Institute
for Advanced Research is a research university without walls.
Established in 1982 by Fraser Mustard, it links more than
170 of the best scholars and researchers from Canada and elsewhere.
CIAR gives these scholars the chance to develop and apply
their work beyond one discipline and to study the effects
of their work in a broader context. The research, particularly
in the social sciences, generally has considerable applied
relevance.
"In many areas of
health and competent functioning there are differences between
countries. Those that have very sharp differences steep
gradients between people at the top and bottom of their
social status scales do worse overall than countries that
have more shallow gradients," says Dan Keating, a professor
at the University of Toronto and chair of the CIAR Human Development
Program.
"These patterns show
up in many ways, including educational achievement and health.
Evidence seems to emerge that the early experiential differences
that are related to social differences do appear to be significantly
implicated in this pattern of outcomes. There are certain
kinds of experiences that help sculpt or shape the brain in
ways that make it easier or harder to do other things later
on.
"In mathematics,"
Keating continues, "as in many other things, what we
do early matters a great deal, probably because the brain
is so plastic early on, and if we deal with them early, we
wind up with much better circumstances later."
CIAR has eight programs:
population health, economic growth and policy, human development,
cosmology and gravity, evolutionary biology, earth system
evolution, soft surfaces and interfaces, and superconductivity.
|
Robbie Case is a Fellow with the Canadian Institute
for Advanced Research and professor at the University of Toronto.
He attended McGill University and received his PhD in education at
OISE (applied psychology). Currently on leave from Stanford University,
he has also taught at OISE and Berkeley.
For a copy of the research, contact the CIAR,
179 John Street, Suite 701, Toronto, ON M5T 1X4. Working papers cost
$5.
For a copy of the 78 lessons and materials, write
to Sharon Griffin, Department of Education, Clark University, 950
Main Street, Worcester, Mass., 01610 for information on when they
will be published.